Kitap İncelemesi
BibTex RIS Kaynak Göster

Kitap Tanıtımı: Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.

Yıl 2020, Cilt: 31 Sayı: 2, 323 - 337, 30.12.2020
https://doi.org/10.18492/dad.841214

Öz

Bu yazıda, dil sisteminin uzaysal ve zamansal işlemlenişini, İşitsel Tümce Anlamlandırmanın Dinamik Çift-Yönlü İşlemleme Modeli (Friederici, 2002, 2011, 2012) çerçevesinde inceleyerek alanyazına çok sayıda araştırmayı kazandıran sinirdilbilimci Angela D. Friederici’nin kaleme aldığı Language in our Brain: The Origins of a Uniquely Human Capacity adlı kitabı tanıtılmaktadır. Avram Noam Chomsky tarafından önsözü yazılmış ve The MIT Press (Cambridge, MA) tarafından 2017’de yayınlanmış olan bu kitap çalışması, dilin biyolojik evrimini, sinirdilbilimsel alt yapısının nasıl bir oluşum sürecinden geçtiğini ve bu sürecini noröbilişsel basamaklarını işitsel ve görsel tabanlı işlemleme modelleri çerçevesinde dilbilimsel bakış açısıyla tartışmaktadır. Dil sisteminin beyindeki işlemlenişi üzerine önemli bir kaynak ve araştırma kitabı niteliği taşıyan bu kitabın ilk bölümlerinde, dilin üretimi ve anlamlandırılmasına yönelik bilişsel süreçlerin sinir ağlarıyla nasıl yapılandığına, bu bileşenlerin beyindeki zamansal işlemlenişi ve uzaysal konumlarına ilişkin kısımlar ve noröbilişsel modeller alanyazından çok sayıda araştırma çerçevesinde yorumlanmaktadır. Kitabın ikinci bölümünde, öbek yapının oluşumuna yönelik anlamlandırma ve üretim tabanlı kısımlar dünya dillerinden örneklerle ele alınırken, kitabının izleyen bölümlerinde yapısal ve işlevsel dil ağları, ön ve arka yolaklar, dil edinimi süreçleri, dilin biyolojik evrimi ve sinirsel ağların işleyişi kapsamlı bir biçimde okuyucuya aktarılmaktadır.

Kaynakça

  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801-2814.
  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23(8), 1859-1873.
  • Bögels, S., Schriefers, H., Vonk, W., Chwilla, D.J. ve Kerkhofs, R. (2010). The interplay between prosody and syntax in sentence processing: The case of subject-and object-control verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1036-1053.
  • Campbell, T., Winkler, I. ve Kujala, T. (2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44 (4): 530–40.
  • Capek, C.M., Grossi, G., Newman, A.J., McBurney, S.L., Corina, D., Roeder, B. ve Neville, H.J. (2009). Brain systems mediating semantic and syntactic processing in deaf native signers: Biological invariance and modality specificity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8784–8789.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E. ve Gout, A. (2000). Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 635-647.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2005). Late interaction of syntactic and prosodic processes in sentence comprehension as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 130-143.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2006). It's early: event-related potential evidence for initial interaction of syntax and prosody in speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(10), 1696-1711.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357-1392.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 262-268.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267-276.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Frisch, S. (2000). Verb-argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 476–507.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain research, 1146, 50-58.
  • Friederici, A.D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R.I. ve Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(7), 2458-2463.
  • Friederici, A.D., Fiebach, C.J., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I.D. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2006). Processing linguistic complexity and grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1709-1717.
  • Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M. ve Weber, C. (2002). Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy. Neuroreport, 13(10), 1251-1254.
  • Friederici, A.D., Meyer, M. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language, 74(2), 289-300.
  • Friederici, A.D., Rüschemeyer, S.A., Hahne, A. ve Fiebach, C.J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177.
  • Goucha, T. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). The language skeleton after dissecting meaning: a functional segregation within Broca's Area. Neuroimage, 114, 294-302.
  • Grewe, T., Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., von Cramon, D.Y. ve Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The emergence of the unmarked: A new perspective on the language‐specific function of Broca's area. Human Brain Mapping, 26(3), 178-190.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (1999). Brain responses during sentence reading: Visual input affects central processes. NeuroReport, 10(15), 3175-3178.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556-568.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439–483.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Osterhout, L. (1999). The neurocognition of syntactic processing. İçinde C.M. Brown ve P. Hagoort (Yay.), The Neurocognition of Language (ss. 273-316). Oxford University Press.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 11(2), 194-205.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2002). Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 339-356.
  • Hartwigsen, G., Golombek, T. ve Obleser, J. (2015). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over left angular gyrus modulates the predictability gain in degraded speech comprehension. Cortex, 68, 100-110.
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An MEG study. Neuroimage, 57(2), 624-633.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 131-138.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67-99.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
  • Hofer, S. ve Frahm, J. (2006). Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited-comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage, 32(3), 989-994.
  • Itzhak, I., Pauker, E., Drury, J.E., Baum, S.R. ve Steinhauer, K. (2010). Event-related potentials show online influence of lexical biases on prosodic processing. NeuroReport, 21(1), 8-13.
  • Kerkhofs, R., Vonk, W., Schriefers, H. ve Chwilla, D.J. (2007). Discourse, syntax, and prosody: The brain reveals an immediate interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1421-1434.
  • Kotz, S.A., Meyer, M., Alter, K., Besson, M., von Cramon, Y. ve Friederici, A.D. (2003). On the lateralization of emotional prosody: An event-related functional MR investigation. Brain and Language, 86(3), 366–376
  • Kutas, M. ve Federmeier, K.D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470.
  • Kutas, M. ve Hillyard, S.A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207 (4427): 203–208.
  • MacSweeney, M., Capek, C.M., Campbell, R. ve Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 432–440.
  • Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20), 8362-8367.
  • McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L. ve Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704.
  • Meyer, M.L., Spunt, R.P., Berkman, E.T., Taylor, S.E. ve Lieberman, M.D. (2012). Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 1883-1888.
  • Mitchell, R.L.C., Elliott, R., Barry, M., Cruttenden, A. ve Woodruff, P.W.R. (2003). The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia, 41(10), 1410–1421.
  • Molinaro, N., Barber, H. A., Caffarra, S. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). On the left anterior negativity (LAN): The case of morphosyntactic agreement. Cortex, 66, 156-159.
  • Molinaro, N., Paz-Alonso, P.M., Duñabeitia, J.A. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). Combinatorial semantics strengthens angular-anterior temporal coupling. Cortex, 65, 113-127.
  • Neubert, F.X., Mars, R.B., Thomas, A.G., Sallet, J. ve Rushworth, M.F.S. (2014). Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700–713.
  • Obleser, J., Elbert, T., Lahiri, A. ve Eulitz, C. (2003). Cortical representation of vowels reflects acoustic dissimilarity determined by formant frequencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(3), 207-213.
  • Osterhout, L. ve Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785–806.
  • Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J. ve Swinney, D.A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786–803.
  • Petersson, K.M., Folia, V. ve Hagoort, P. (2012). What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language, 120(2), 83-95.
  • Petersson, K.M., Forkstam, C. ve Ingvar, M. (2004). Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region. Cognitive Science, 28(3), 383-407.
  • Phillips, C., Pellathy, T., Marantz, A., Yellin, E., Wexler, K., Poeppel, D., McGinnis, M. ve Roberts, T. (2000). Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1038–55.
  • Rilling, J.K., Glasser, M.F., Preuss, T.M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X. ve Behrens, T.E.J. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 426–428.
  • Rogalsky, C. ve Hickok, G. (2011). The role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664-1680.
  • Rossi, S., Gugler, M.F., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048.
  • Sammler, D., Kotz, S.A., Eckstein, K., Ott, D.V. ve Friederici, A.D. (2010). Prosody meets syntax: The role of the corpus callosum. Brain, 133(9), 2643-2655.
  • Santi, A. ve Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). fMRI adaptation dissociates syntactic complexity dimensions. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1285-1293.
  • Silva-Pereyra, J.F. ve Carreiras, M. (2007). An ERP study of agreement features in Spanish. Brain Research, 1185, 201-211.
  • Skeide, M.A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 17, 323–332.
  • Skeide, M.A., Brauer, J. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex, 26(5), 2127–2139.
  • Steinhauer, K. ve Drury, J.E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162.
  • Steinhauer, K., Alter, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2(2), 191-196.
  • Tyler, L.K., Meredith, A.S., Randall, B., Wright, B., Marslen-Wilson, W.D. ve Stamatakis, E.A. (2010). Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2, 352–364.
  • Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A.C. ve Price, C.J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550-560.
  • Wildgruber, D., Riecker, A., Hertrich, I., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Ethofer, T. ve Ackermann, H. (2005). Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI. NeuroImage, 24(4), 1233–1241.
  • Wolff, S., Schlesewsky, M., Hirotani, M. ve Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2008). The neural mechanisms of word order processing revisited: Electrophysiological evidence from Japanese. Brain and Language, 107(2), 133-157.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). Merge in the human brain: A sub-region based functional investigation in the left pars opercularis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1818.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The neuroanatomical network of syntactic merge for language. MPI CBS, Research Report, 2014–2016 (p. 68). http://www.cbs.mpg.de/institute/research-reports.

Book Review: Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.

Yıl 2020, Cilt: 31 Sayı: 2, 323 - 337, 30.12.2020
https://doi.org/10.18492/dad.841214

Öz

Kaynakça

  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801-2814.
  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23(8), 1859-1873.
  • Bögels, S., Schriefers, H., Vonk, W., Chwilla, D.J. ve Kerkhofs, R. (2010). The interplay between prosody and syntax in sentence processing: The case of subject-and object-control verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1036-1053.
  • Campbell, T., Winkler, I. ve Kujala, T. (2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44 (4): 530–40.
  • Capek, C.M., Grossi, G., Newman, A.J., McBurney, S.L., Corina, D., Roeder, B. ve Neville, H.J. (2009). Brain systems mediating semantic and syntactic processing in deaf native signers: Biological invariance and modality specificity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8784–8789.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E. ve Gout, A. (2000). Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 635-647.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2005). Late interaction of syntactic and prosodic processes in sentence comprehension as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 130-143.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2006). It's early: event-related potential evidence for initial interaction of syntax and prosody in speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(10), 1696-1711.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357-1392.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 262-268.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267-276.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Frisch, S. (2000). Verb-argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 476–507.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain research, 1146, 50-58.
  • Friederici, A.D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R.I. ve Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(7), 2458-2463.
  • Friederici, A.D., Fiebach, C.J., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I.D. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2006). Processing linguistic complexity and grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1709-1717.
  • Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M. ve Weber, C. (2002). Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy. Neuroreport, 13(10), 1251-1254.
  • Friederici, A.D., Meyer, M. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language, 74(2), 289-300.
  • Friederici, A.D., Rüschemeyer, S.A., Hahne, A. ve Fiebach, C.J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177.
  • Goucha, T. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). The language skeleton after dissecting meaning: a functional segregation within Broca's Area. Neuroimage, 114, 294-302.
  • Grewe, T., Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., von Cramon, D.Y. ve Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The emergence of the unmarked: A new perspective on the language‐specific function of Broca's area. Human Brain Mapping, 26(3), 178-190.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (1999). Brain responses during sentence reading: Visual input affects central processes. NeuroReport, 10(15), 3175-3178.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556-568.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439–483.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Osterhout, L. (1999). The neurocognition of syntactic processing. İçinde C.M. Brown ve P. Hagoort (Yay.), The Neurocognition of Language (ss. 273-316). Oxford University Press.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 11(2), 194-205.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2002). Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 339-356.
  • Hartwigsen, G., Golombek, T. ve Obleser, J. (2015). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over left angular gyrus modulates the predictability gain in degraded speech comprehension. Cortex, 68, 100-110.
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An MEG study. Neuroimage, 57(2), 624-633.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 131-138.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67-99.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
  • Hofer, S. ve Frahm, J. (2006). Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited-comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage, 32(3), 989-994.
  • Itzhak, I., Pauker, E., Drury, J.E., Baum, S.R. ve Steinhauer, K. (2010). Event-related potentials show online influence of lexical biases on prosodic processing. NeuroReport, 21(1), 8-13.
  • Kerkhofs, R., Vonk, W., Schriefers, H. ve Chwilla, D.J. (2007). Discourse, syntax, and prosody: The brain reveals an immediate interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1421-1434.
  • Kotz, S.A., Meyer, M., Alter, K., Besson, M., von Cramon, Y. ve Friederici, A.D. (2003). On the lateralization of emotional prosody: An event-related functional MR investigation. Brain and Language, 86(3), 366–376
  • Kutas, M. ve Federmeier, K.D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470.
  • Kutas, M. ve Hillyard, S.A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207 (4427): 203–208.
  • MacSweeney, M., Capek, C.M., Campbell, R. ve Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 432–440.
  • Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20), 8362-8367.
  • McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L. ve Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704.
  • Meyer, M.L., Spunt, R.P., Berkman, E.T., Taylor, S.E. ve Lieberman, M.D. (2012). Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 1883-1888.
  • Mitchell, R.L.C., Elliott, R., Barry, M., Cruttenden, A. ve Woodruff, P.W.R. (2003). The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia, 41(10), 1410–1421.
  • Molinaro, N., Barber, H. A., Caffarra, S. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). On the left anterior negativity (LAN): The case of morphosyntactic agreement. Cortex, 66, 156-159.
  • Molinaro, N., Paz-Alonso, P.M., Duñabeitia, J.A. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). Combinatorial semantics strengthens angular-anterior temporal coupling. Cortex, 65, 113-127.
  • Neubert, F.X., Mars, R.B., Thomas, A.G., Sallet, J. ve Rushworth, M.F.S. (2014). Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700–713.
  • Obleser, J., Elbert, T., Lahiri, A. ve Eulitz, C. (2003). Cortical representation of vowels reflects acoustic dissimilarity determined by formant frequencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(3), 207-213.
  • Osterhout, L. ve Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785–806.
  • Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J. ve Swinney, D.A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786–803.
  • Petersson, K.M., Folia, V. ve Hagoort, P. (2012). What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language, 120(2), 83-95.
  • Petersson, K.M., Forkstam, C. ve Ingvar, M. (2004). Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region. Cognitive Science, 28(3), 383-407.
  • Phillips, C., Pellathy, T., Marantz, A., Yellin, E., Wexler, K., Poeppel, D., McGinnis, M. ve Roberts, T. (2000). Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1038–55.
  • Rilling, J.K., Glasser, M.F., Preuss, T.M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X. ve Behrens, T.E.J. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 426–428.
  • Rogalsky, C. ve Hickok, G. (2011). The role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664-1680.
  • Rossi, S., Gugler, M.F., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048.
  • Sammler, D., Kotz, S.A., Eckstein, K., Ott, D.V. ve Friederici, A.D. (2010). Prosody meets syntax: The role of the corpus callosum. Brain, 133(9), 2643-2655.
  • Santi, A. ve Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). fMRI adaptation dissociates syntactic complexity dimensions. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1285-1293.
  • Silva-Pereyra, J.F. ve Carreiras, M. (2007). An ERP study of agreement features in Spanish. Brain Research, 1185, 201-211.
  • Skeide, M.A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 17, 323–332.
  • Skeide, M.A., Brauer, J. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex, 26(5), 2127–2139.
  • Steinhauer, K. ve Drury, J.E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162.
  • Steinhauer, K., Alter, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2(2), 191-196.
  • Tyler, L.K., Meredith, A.S., Randall, B., Wright, B., Marslen-Wilson, W.D. ve Stamatakis, E.A. (2010). Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2, 352–364.
  • Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A.C. ve Price, C.J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550-560.
  • Wildgruber, D., Riecker, A., Hertrich, I., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Ethofer, T. ve Ackermann, H. (2005). Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI. NeuroImage, 24(4), 1233–1241.
  • Wolff, S., Schlesewsky, M., Hirotani, M. ve Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2008). The neural mechanisms of word order processing revisited: Electrophysiological evidence from Japanese. Brain and Language, 107(2), 133-157.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). Merge in the human brain: A sub-region based functional investigation in the left pars opercularis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1818.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The neuroanatomical network of syntactic merge for language. MPI CBS, Research Report, 2014–2016 (p. 68). http://www.cbs.mpg.de/institute/research-reports.
Toplam 69 adet kaynakça vardır.

Ayrıntılar

Birincil Dil Türkçe
Bölüm Kitap Tanıtımları
Yazarlar

İpek Pınar Uzun 0000-0003-3103-0758

Yayımlanma Tarihi 30 Aralık 2020
Yayımlandığı Sayı Yıl 2020Cilt: 31 Sayı: 2

Kaynak Göster

APA Uzun, İ. P. (2020). Kitap Tanıtımı: Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi, 31(2), 323-337. https://doi.org/10.18492/dad.841214